Comparing ethical self-superiority in the fields of doing ethical behaviors and not doing unethical behaviors

Document Type : Research Paper

Author

Department of Psychology, Buinzahra Branch, Islamic Azad University, Buinzahra, Iran

Abstract

The aim of this study was to compare the ethical self-superiority in terms of doing good deeds and in terms of not doing bad deeds. moral self-superiority has two forms: (1) the belief that "I do good things more than others", and (2) the belief that "I do bad things less than others", and the intensity of moral self-superiority can be different in these two areas. The research design was experimental and within-subjects and the research population were students of Islamic Azad University of Buin Zahra in 2016-17 academic year (N=5000) among which 73 were selected using random sampling. The research tool was a researcher-made scale in which the subjects were asked to determine whether they or others are more likely to do seven moral and immoral behaviors. One-sample t-test showed that when the subjects are confronted with unethical behaviors, they confirm that they are less likely than others to perform the behaviors (self-superiority), but when they encounter ethical behaviors, they do not announce that they are more likely than others to perform the behaviors (do not have self-superiority).

Keywords


Bahrami, H., Asadi, M., Shiralipoor, A. (2011). The relationship between identity, self-efficiency, and fundamental needs with general health in school students: A fundamental correlation. Applied psychological research quarterly, 1 (2): 29-44 (in persian).
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 243–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243
Bocchiaro, P., Zimbardo, P. G., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2012). To defy or not to defy: An experimental study of the dynamics of disobedience and whistle-blowing. Social Influence, 7, 35–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15534510.2011.648421
Campbell, W. K., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of General Psychology, 3, 23–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.3.1.23
Chambers, J. R., & Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social comparative judgments: The role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 813–838. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813
Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 377–383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0025589
Dawson, E., Gilovich, T., & Regan, D. T. (2002). Motivated reasoning and performance on the Wason selection task. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1379 –1387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014616702236869
Ditto, P. H., Scepansky, J. A., Munro, G. D., Apanovitch, A. M., & Lockhart, L. K. (1998). Motivated sensitivity to preference-inconsistent information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 53–69.
Effron, D. A. (2014). Making mountains of morality from molehills of virtue: Threat causes people to overestimate their moral credentials. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 972–985. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167214533131
Epley, N., & Dunning, D. (2000). Feeling “holier than thou”: Are selfserving assessments produced by errors in self- or social prediction? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 861–875. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.86
Flynn, F. J., & Lake, V. K. B. (2008). If you need help, just ask: Underestimating compliance with direct requests for help. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 128–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.128
Gilbert, D. T., Lieberman, M. D., Morewedge, C. K., & Wilson, T. D. (2004). The peculiar longevity of things not so bad. Psychological Science, 15, 14 –19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501003.x
Helzer, E. G., & Dunning, D. (2012). Why and when peer prediction is superior to self-prediction: The weight given to future aspiration versus past achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103,38–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028124
Kawakami, K., Dunn, E., Karmali, F., & Dovidio, J. F. (2009, January 9). Mispredicting affective and behavioral responses to racism. Science, 323, 276–278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1164951
Khodayarifard, M., Ghobaribonab, B., Zamanpoor, E., Derakhshan, M., Akbari, Z., Moosavi, S. (2014). Optimism: Developing an Iranian-Islamic paradigm. Applied psychological research quarterly, 3 (3): 13-23 (in persian). Klein, N., & Epley, N. (2016). Maybe holier, but definitely less evil, than you: Bounded self-righteousness in social judgment. Journal of personality and social psychology, 110(5), 660.
Koehler, D. J., & Poon, C. S. K. (2006). Self-predictions overweight strength of current intentions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 517–524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.08.003
Kruger, J., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Actions, intentions, and self-assessment: The road to self-enhancement is paved with good intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 328–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203259932
Messick, D. M., Bloom, S., Boldizar, J. P., & Samuelson, C. D. (1985). Why we are fairer than others. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 480–500. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(85)90031-9
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0040525
Moore, D. A., & Small, D. A. (2007). Error and bias in comparative judgment: On being both better and worse than we think we are. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 972–989. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.972
Myers, D. G. (2010). Social psychology (10th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Pronin, E., & Kugler, M. B. (2007). Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 565–578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.011
RahimineZhad, A., Rahimi Jafari, M., Salamati, P. (2016). The relationship between identity styles and cognitive-social-emotional wellbeing: the mediating role of
commitment and accepting religious identity. Applied psychological research quarterly, 3 (1): 95-112 (in persian).
Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 296–320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 657–661. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2213
Williams, E. F., Gilovich, T., & Dunning, D. (2012). Being all that you can be: The weighting of potential in assessments of self and others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 143–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167211421937
Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People’s appraisals of their earlier and present selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 572–584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.572
Volume 8, Issue 1
August 2017
Pages 157-168
  • Receive Date: 04 January 2017
  • Revise Date: 14 October 2017
  • Accept Date: 09 March 2017
  • First Publish Date: 21 April 2017
  • Publish Date: 21 April 2017