The Effect of Concrete versus Abstract Processing Mode on Working Memory Capacity in High Rumination

Document Type : Research Paper

Authors

1 MSc. Student, Department of Psychology, psychology and Educational Science Faculty, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran

2 Department of Psychology, psychology and Educational Science Faculty, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran

3 Department of Educational Psychology and Counseling, psychology and Educational Science Faculty, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran

Abstract

The purpose of this study is to investigate the effects of Concrete Processing Mode (CPM) and Abstract Processing Mode (APM) on working memory capacity during high rumination. The research method of this basic study is experimental. The statistical population consists of individuals aged 18-50 years with high rumination living in Tehran who voluntarily participated in the study in January 2021. Among them, 30 individuals (21 women and 9 men) with high rumination were selected using the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS) and the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). Participants were randomly divided into two groups of 15 people each, a concrete group and an abstract group, who were put in a negative mood by watching a sad movie. They then performed the instructions CPM or APM, depending on the group. Participants' working memory was measured with the Reading Span Test (RST) before they watched the movie and after they performed the processing mode instruction. The results of the covariance analysis of the data using SPSS version 26 software showed that the CPM group had significantly higher working memory capacity than the APM group. This meant that the capacity of working memory was affected by the changes in processing mode. This effect was evident on the working memory processing subscale, but the changes on the working memory storage subscale did not differ significantly between groups. Consistent with previous findings, CPM was also considered an adaptive mode in contrast to APM in this study. The results showed that processing mode can predict changes in working memory capacity during severe rumination.

Keywords


اسدزاده، ح. (1388). بررسی رابطه ظرفیت حافظة فعال و عملکرد تحصیلی دانش آموزان سوم راهنمایی شهر تهران. تعلیم و تربیت. 97(25)، 70-53.
فتی، ل.، بیرشک، ب.، عاطف‌وحید، م. ک.، و استفان دابسون، ک. (1384). ساختارهای معناگذاری/طرح‌واره‌ها، حالت‌های هیجانی و پردازش شناختی اطلاعات هیجانی: مقایسة دو چارچوب مفهومی. مجلة روان‌پزشکی و روا‌ن‌شناسی بالینی ایران. 11(3)، 326-312.
فرخی، ح.، سیدزاده، ا.، و مصطفی‌پور، و. (1396). بررسی ساختار عاملی، اعتبار و پایایی فرم فارسی مقیاس باورهای مثبت و منفی نشخوار فکری (PBRS-NBRS) و مقیاس پاسخ نشخوار فکری (RRS). مجلة پیشرفت‌های نوین در علوم رفتاری. 2(14)، 37-21.
Altamirano, L. J., Miyake, A., & Whitmer, A. J. (2010). When mental inflexibility facilitates executive control: Beneficial side effects of ruminative tendencies on goal maintenance. Psychological Science21(10), 1377-1382.
Antrobus, J. S. (1968). Information theory and stimulus‐independent thought. British Journal of Psychology59(4), 423-430.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences4(11), 417-423.
Bassanini, A., Caselli, G., Fiore, F., Ruggiero, G. M., Sassaroli, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2014). Why “why” seems better than “how”. Processes underlining repetitive thinking in an Italian non-clinical sample. Personality and Individual Differences64(4), 18-23.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation
Blagden, J. C., & Craske, M. G. (1996). Effects of active and passive rumination and distraction: A pilot replication with anxious mood. Journal of Anxiety Disorders10(4), 243-252.
Bushman, B. J. (2002). Does venting anger feed or extinguish the flame? Catharsis, rumination, distraction, anger, and aggressive responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin28(6), 724-731.
Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N. (2005). Chewing on it can chew you up: Effects of rumination on triggered displaced aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology88(6), 969-983.
Bushman, B. J., & Gibson, B. (2011). Violent video games cause an increase in aggression long after the game has been turned off. Social Psychological and Personality Science2(1), 29-32.
Carriedo, N., Elosúa, M. R., & García-Madruga, J. A. (2011). Working memory, text comprehension, and propositional reasoning: A new semantic anaphora WM test. The Spanish Journal of Psychology14(1), 37-49.‌
Daamen, J., van Vugt, M. K., & Taatgen, N. (2016). Measuring and modeling distraction by self-referential processing in a complex working memory span task. Cognitive Science Society. 31(1), 2147-2152.‌
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior19(4), 450-466.
Davis, R. N., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). Cognitive inflexibility among ruminators and nonruminators. Cognitive Therapy and Research24(6), 699-711.
De Lissnyder, E., Koster, E. H., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2010). The association between depressive symptoms and executive control impairments in response to emotional and non-emotional information. Cognition and Emotion24(2), 264-280.
Donaldson, C., Lam, D., & Mathews, A. (2007). Rumination and attention in major depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy45(11), 2664-2678.‌
Ehring, T., Frank, S., & Ehlers, A. (2008). The role of rumination and reduced concreteness in the maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma. Cognitive Therapy and Research32(4), 488-506.
Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion7(2), 336-353.‌
Gotlib, I. H., & Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology6(1), 285-312.
Hayes M. H. S., & Patterson, D. G. (1921). Experimental development of the graphic rating method. Psychol. Bull. 18(3), 98–99.
Hertel, P. T. (1998). Relation between rumination and impaired memory in dysphoric moods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology107(1), 166-173.
Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Gollwitzer, M., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2005). A revised film set for the induction of basic emotions. Cognition and Emotion19(7), 1095-1110.
Joormann, J. (2006). Differential effects of rumination and dysphoria on the inhibition of irrelevant emotional material: Evidence from a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research30(2), 149-160.
Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumination? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy37(3), 269-280.
Joormann, J., & Vanderlind, W. M. (2014). Emotion regulation in depression: The role of biased cognition and reduced cognitive control. Clinical Psychological Science2(4), 402-421.‌
Klein, K., & Boals, A. (2001). The relationship of life event stress and working memory capacity. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition15(5), 565-579.
Koster, E. H., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & De Raedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: The impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review31(1), 138-145.
‌ Koster, E. H., Hoorelbeke, K., Onraedt, T., Owens, M., & Derakshan, N. (2017). Cognitive control interventions for depression: A systematic review of findings from training studies. Clinical Psychology Review53(3), 79-92.‌
Levens, S. M., Muhtadie, L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Rumination and impaired resource allocation in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology118(4), 757-766.
Lyubomirsky, S., Kasri, F., & Zehm, K. (2003). Dysphoric rumination impairs concentration on academic tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research27(3), 309-330.
Lyubomirsky, S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1995). Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology69(1), 176-190.
Luminet, O. (2004). Measurement of depressive rumination and associated constructs. In C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination (pp. 187-215). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
 MacLeod, C., Rutherford, E., Campbell, L., Ebsworthy, G., & Holker, L. (2002). Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: Assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology111(1), 107-123.
Marchetti, I., Mor, N., Chiorri, C., & Koster, E. H. (2018). The brief state rumination inventory (BSRI): Validation and psychometric evaluation. Cognitive Therapy and Research42(4), 447-460.
Meiran, N., Diamond, G. M., Toder, D., & Nemets, B. (2011). Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: Examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry Research185(1-2), 149-156.
Muris, P., Roelofs, J., Rassin, E., Franken, I., & Mayer, B. (2005). Mediating effects of rumination and worry on the links between neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Personality and Individual Differences39(6), 1105-1111.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 100(4), 569-582.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology102(1), 20-28.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Parker, L. E., & Larson, J. (1994). Ruminative coping with depressed mood following loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology67(1), 92-104.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science3(5), 400-424.
Offredi, A., Caselli, G., Manfredi, C., Ruggiero, G. M., Sassaroli, S., Liuzzo, P., & Rovetto, F. (2016). Effects of anger rumination on different scenarios of anger: An experimental investigation. American Journal of Psychology129(4), 381-390.
Onraedt, T., & Koster, E. H. (2014). Training working memory to reduce rumination. PloS One. 9(3), https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090632.
Owens, M., Koster, E. H., & Derakshan, N. (2012). Impaired filtering of irrelevant information in dysphoria: An ERP study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience7(7), 752-763.
Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2001). Positive beliefs about depressive rumination: Development and preliminary validation of a self-report scale. Behavior Therapy32(1), 13-26.
Pelosi, L., Slade, T., Blumhardt, L. D., & Sharma, V. K. (2000). Working memory dysfunction in major depression: An event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology111(9), 1531-1543.
Pezzulo, G. (2007). Working memory. Institute of Cognitive Science and Technology-CNR, Roma. Retrieved from http://cogsci.ucd.ie/introtocogsci/docs/ working_memory.pdf
Philippot, P., & Brutoux, F. (2008). Induced rumination dampens executive processes in dysphoric young adults. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry39(3), 219-227.
Rapee, R. M. (1993). The utilisation of working memory by worry. Behaviour Research and Therapy31(6), 617-620.
Rimes, K. A., & Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global negative self-judgements in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy43(12), 1673-1681.
Rippere, V. (1977). What's the thing to do when you're feeling depressed: A pilot study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 15(2), 185–191.
Rose, E. J., & Ebmeier, K. (2006). Pattern of impaired working memory during major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 90(2-3), 149-161.
Rosenbaum, D., Haipt, A., Fuhr, K., Haeussinger, F. B., Metzger, F. G., Nuerk, H. C., …, & Ehlis, A. C. (2017). Aberrant functional connectivity in depression as an index of state and trait rumination. Scientific Reports7(1), 1-12.
Rusting, C. L., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses to anger: Effects of rumination and distraction on angry mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology74(3), 790-803.
Sari, B. A., Koster, E. H., & Derakshan, N. (2017). The effects of active worrying on working memory capacity. Cognition and Emotion31(5), 995-1003.‌
Schaefer, A., Nils, F., Sanchez, X., & Philippot, P. (2010). Assessing the effectiveness of a large database of emotion-eliciting films: A new tool for emotion researchers. Cognition and Emotion24(7), 1153-1172.
Siegle, G. J., Ghinassi, F., & Thase, M. E. (2007). Neurobehavioral therapies in the 21st century: Summary of an emerging field and an extended example of cognitive control training for depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research31(2), 235-262.‌
Siegle, G. J., Price, R. B., Jones, N. P., Ghinassi, F., Painter, T., & Thase, M. E. (2014). You gotta work at it: Pupillary indices of task focus are prognostic for response to a neurocognitive intervention for rumination in depression. Clinical Psychological Science2(4), 455-471.‌
Stavropoulos, A., & Berle, D. (2020). The influence of ruminative processing mode on the trajectory of intrusive memories following a negative mood induction. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 68(3), 101528.
Stoltzfus, E. R., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1996). Working memory and aging: Current status of the inhibitory view. In  J. T. E. Richardson (Ed.), Working memory and human cognition (pp. 66-88). New York: Oxford University Press.
Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Memory & Cognition23(5), 551-559.
Teasdale, J. D., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and stimulus-independent thought: Effects of memory load and presentation rate. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology5(4), 417-433.
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research27(3), 247-259.‌
Vasquez, E. A., Pedersen, W. C., Bushman, B. J., Kelley, N. J., Demeestere, P., & Miller, N. (2013). Lashing out after stewing over public insults: The effects of public provocation, provocation intensity, and rumination on triggered displaced aggression. Aggressive Behavior39(1), 13-29.
Wanmaker, S., Geraerts, E., & Franken, I. H. (2015). A working memory training to decrease rumination in depressed and anxious individuals: A double-blind randomized controlled trial. Journal of Affective Disorders. 175(6), 310-319.
Watkins, E. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive ruminative self-focus during emotional processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy42(9), 1037-1052.
Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin134(2), 163-206.
Watkins, E. R. (2018). Rumination-focused cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. New York: Guilford Publications.
‌Watkins, E., & Brown, R. G. (2002). Rumination and executive function in depression: An experimental study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry72(3), 400-402.
Watkins, E., Moberly, N. J., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing mode causally influences emotional reactivity: distinct effects of abstract versus concrete construal on emotional response. Emotion8(3), 364-378.
Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: Impact of abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion. 5(3), 319-328.
Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. L. (2007). Reduced concreteness of rumination in depression: A pilot study. Personality and Individual Differences43(6), 1386-1395.
Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and overgeneral memory in depression: Effects of self-focus and analytic thinking. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 110(2), 353-357.
Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 82(1), 1-8.
Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). An attentional scope model of rumination. Psychological Bulletin139(5), 1036-1061.‌
Wisco, B. E., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2008). Ruminative response style. In K. S. Dobson, & D. Dozois (Eds.), Risk factors in depression (pp. 221-236). New York: Elsevier.
 
Volume 12, Issue 4
February 2022
Pages 271-291
  • Receive Date: 07 July 2021
  • Revise Date: 06 September 2021
  • Accept Date: 08 September 2021
  • First Publish Date: 26 January 2022
  • Publish Date: 20 February 2022